Friday, April 29, 2022

Medium scores for readings: Beischel excerpt #11

The scores for target readings reflect the accuracy of the information; the comparison of target reading scores to decoy reading scores reflects its specificity. If the mediums are just making up the content or guessing, we would expect to see very low accuracy scores. If the information is overly general and could apply to any sitter, we would expect to see high accuracy scores with no differences between the target and decoy scores.

What we found was that, in general, the blinded sitters in this study scored readings— performed by blinded mediums—for the sitters’ own discarnates (targets) as more accurate than readings for other sitters’ discarnates (decoys).

The statistically significant scoring data collected under blinded conditions reflect the accuracy and specificity of the information the mediums reported and are in line with the original hypothesis. Stated more plainly: The mediums in this experiment reported accurate information about deceased people that they had no way of knowing.

The data collected during this study demonstrate the phenomenon we call anomalous information reception (AIR), that is, the reporting by mediums of accurate and specific information about discarnates without prior knowledge of the discarnates or sitters, in the absence of any sensory feedback, and without using deceptive or fraudulent means. “There’s no normal way the mediums could acquire the information they report so its reception can only be described as anomalous” (that is, not normal; inconsistent with what is standard or expected). This phenomenon is not possible within the currently prevailing scientific or medical paradigms. And we use the term “reception” rather than “retrieval” to reflect the lived experiences of the mediums who report receiving or perceiving rather than retrieving the information.

Anomalous or not, these are not fluke findings. A meta-analysis of 14 studies of mediums’ accuracy published since 2001 was recently conducted. The method of meta- analysis (MA) incorporates an effective array of tools for combining data across studies and addressing controversial research findings. This particular MA also included publication bias tests to examine biases resulting from questionable research practices. The authors confirmed the reliability of the results from the studies analyzed and concluded that “some mediums are able to acquire information about deceased persons through some unknown or anomalous means”. So, the current status of the field is that at least some mediums are capable of AIR.

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php). 

Thursday, April 28, 2022

Experimental data: Beischel excerpt #10

Step 4: Perform the Experiment

Between 2009 and 2013, the 20 WCRMs on my team performed 68 quintuple-blind readings for 68 discarnates. From these my colleagues and I received 58 readings scored by sitters that contained usable data.

The main features of the quintuple-blind protocol are presented in Figure 1.


Step 5: Analyze the Data


In 2015, my colleagues and I published the results from the 58 quintuple-blind readings (48). The blinded sitters had provided overall global scores (on a 0-6 scale) for each of two readings (a target intended for them and a decoy intended for another sitter) and chose which reading of the two they believed was theirs. For 31 of those readings, the sitters also scored for accuracy each item in the portions of each of the two readings in which the mediums answered specific questions about the deceased. The scores that sitters could choose from included: obvious fit that does not require interpretation to apply, fit requiring minimal interpretation or symbolism to make sense, indirect fit requiring greater interpretation to fit, and complete miss. ‘Fits’ and ‘fits requiring minimal interpretation’ were grouped together and considered ‘hits.’ 

 

Note: The five levels of blinding were: (1) the WCRM was blinded to information about the sitter and the discarnate before, during, and after the reading and asked questions during the reading about the discarnate’s appearance, personality, activities, and cause of death; (2) the blinded sitters did not hear the readings as they occurred; they scored blinded transcripts of two readings, one for their discarnate (target) and one for another sitter’s discarnate (decoy) without knowing which was which; (3) the experimenter who consented and trained the sitters (Experimenter 1) was blinded to which mediums read which sitters and which readings were intended for which sitters; (4) the experimenter who interacted with the mediums during the phone readings and formatted the readings into item lists for scoring (Experimenter 2) was blinded to information about the sitters and the discarnates; (5) the experimenter who interacted with the sitters during scoring (Experimenter 3) was blinded to all information about the discarnates, to which medium performed which readings, and to which readings were intended for which sitters.

 

The scores for target readings reflect the accuracy of the information; the comparison of target reading scores to decoy reading scores reflects its specificity. If the mediums are just making up the content or guessing, we would expect to see very low accuracy scores. If the information is overly general and could apply to any sitter, we would expect to see high accuracy scores with no differences between the target and decoy scores. 

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).



Wednesday, April 27, 2022

Reading and controls: Beischel excerpt #9

Julie Beischel writes in “Beyond Reasonable: Scientific Evidence for Survival,” her prize-winning essay in the Bigelow Institute for Consciousness Studies competition:

Reading. During any research reading, we need to ensure that we only ask the mediums to report the types of information they usually report. Since this does not include winning lottery numbers, combinations to locks, or what color shirt the sitter should wear tomorrow, I didn’t ask for any of those things in my experiments. Additionally, although in your physical life you are regularly known by your personally-identifiable information (PII), like your name, date of birth, social security number, address, and phone number, these are not the types of information mediums are regularly observed reporting, so I didn’t ask for those during research. To further optimize the environment, I needed to limit both the number and length of readings in order to best mimic the real-world experiences of practicing mediums.

Maximum Controls. In order to control for artifacts, I needed to implement maximum experimental controls. If I wanted to study how a seed grows naturally, I would need to control for things like fertilizer and supplemental UV lamps or I’d end up making errors in my understanding of plant growth. In mediumship research, we need to address the normal, sensory explanations for the source of the information the mediums report. These include factors like hot and cold readings.

The Windbridge Research Center offers descriptions of how a fraudulent medium uses these to fabricate accurate readings:

With hot reading, the fraud will obtain information about the sitter beforehand and feed it back to them during the reading and act like it’s coming from the deceased. The information can be looked up online through social media or using confederates onsite to chat up the sitter. With cold reading, the fraud asks the sitter questions and uses their responses or uses sensory clues or cues (for example, the name, age, or gender of the sitter, their clothing or accent, tears, gasps, nods, pupil dilation, the sitter smelling of cigarette smoke, etc.) to steer the direction of the reading. A reading containing information so general it could apply to nearly anyone is also a form of cold reading.

In addition to hot and cold reading, some sitters may have a cognitive tendency to remember many items as accurate even when they were incorrect or unclear. When a sitter knows a reading was intended for them, they may rate or score items differently. During research, this ‘rater bias’ can be responsible for what seems like an accurate reading. Finally, another possible explanation for a medium’s accuracy is precognition: that is, the medium may obtain, from the future, information about which items in the reading were scored as accurate by the rater when the medium is given feedback about the scored reading.

To address these explanations for the information mediums report, the Windbridge protocol uses five levels of experimental blinding (also called masking). In research, blinding refers to the act of preventing people associated with an experiment from knowing certain pieces or types of information. For example, in a standard randomized controlled trial (RCT) involving double-blinding, the patient and the doctor are both blinded to whether the patient is taking a placebo or the medication being studied. In what we have nicknamed our “quintuple-blinded” mediumship protocol, the medium, the sitter, and three experimenters are blinded to various aspects of the protocol and to different types of information. This does not mean that anyone is blindfolded or gets poked in the eye to ensure experimental constraints. It just means that access to information is controlled.

To create the quintuple-blinding, our research readings involve only phone readings, and the sitter is not on the call. An experimenter blinded to information about the sitter and their associated discarnate serves as a proxy sitter in place of the absent sitter. Because the medium has no access to the sitter, and the blinded experimenter cannot provide cues or clues, this protocol addresses the hot and cold reading explanations. In addition, the experimenter asks the medium specific questions about the discarnate’s physical and personality characteristics, hobbies, cause of death, and any messages for the absent sitter. By asking for specific information, this addresses the overly general information explanation.

Furthermore, each medium performs two readings for two different discarnates, and then each associated sitter scores formatted transcripts of both readings without knowing which was intended for them (the target reading) and which was intended for another sitter (the decoy reading). The protocol addresses rater bias by comparing the accuracy scores of all the target readings with the accuracy scores of all the decoy readings. To address precognition, we never give the research mediums feedback about their research readings. Finally, each reading contains information about only one discarnate. This prevents cues that multiple-discarnate readings may provide to the raters.

This quintuple-blind protocol was vetted and peer-reviewed at least four times by multiple qualified peers: first, when a description of the planned project was selected to be funded by the funding organization’s reviewers; second, when the final report about the study findings was reviewed by the funding organization; third, when a description of the protocol and findings was vetted and accepted for presentation at a scientific conference; and fourth, when an article describing the findings was reviewed for publication in a journal.

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).


Monday, April 25, 2022

Discarnates/Sitters/Mediums: Beischel excerpt #8

Julie Beischel writes in “Beyond Reasonable: Scientific Evidence for Survival,” her prize-winning essay in the Bigelow Institute for Consciousness Studies competition:

Discarnates. The observed phenomenon involves everyday discarnates and a close sitter-discarnate relationship. In a valid research design, I can’t ask a medium for information about, say, Nobel prize-winning physicist Richard Feynman. I did not have a close personal relationship with him—and don’t know anyone who did—so I would not be able to identify him during a mediumship reading. Also, the mediums’ lived experience is that the right discarnates find them and not vice versa. They don’t have a Heaven phonebook to ring up whoever they want. They connect discarnates with sitters. Because I have no connection to discarnate Professor Feynman, there’s no logical reason to think he would want to connect with me during a mediumship reading. So, I stuck with everyday discarnates and their everyday sitter loved ones in my design.

Sitters. To fully establish an optimal mediumship research environment, we need to include the sitters in the experiment. The information reported in a mediumship reading is a personal conversation between two people with an emotional connection: the discarnate and the sitter. As I have previously noted elsewhere, even if I asked you to tell me everything there is to know about your deceased loved one, truly meaningful information may still come up in a mediumship reading that you hadn’t thought about in years. If I tried to act as an independent judge and score that forgotten information based on what I had collected from you, I would erroneously label it as inaccurate. Only you can decide what is identifying and accurate about your discarnate. It doesn’t matter what any of your friends, what any skeptic, or even what I think; it is a reading for you, not for any of us. Only people who were close to the discarnate are qualified to assess the accuracy and meaning of a reading. Thus, in my experimental design, I was only concerned with accuracy scores provided by sitters. That’s how it works in a natural, regular reading.

Mediums. The experimental design must also include people capable of the tasks requested of them. This involves pre-screening participants. If we wanted to study the phenomenon of high jumping, we would find some good high jumpers. We wouldn’t just look for people claiming to be high jumpers on Craig’s List, or invite some people off the street and tell them, “Go jump over that bar.” If those people couldn’t clear the bar, we wouldn’t have learned anything at all about high jumping.

In 2008, my team was fortunate enough to receive a research grant to establish a squad of credentialed mediums to participate in research. These mediums were tested, screened, and trained over several months using an intensive, peer-reviewed, 8-step procedure. Upon successful completion of the eight steps, mediums are termed Windbridge Certified Research Mediums (WCRMs). WCRMs agree to volunteer their time as research participants. This includes giving me feedback about protocol designs as well as participating in studies. Because certification is a time- and resource-intensive process, we stopped certifying mediums after the granted project was complete.

At its maximum, my team included 20 WCRMs. After the retirement of a couple, our current team includes 18 WCRMs. The reference I made above to “some mediums I know” who provided responses to the question “Do you believe in an afterlife?” are some of the WCRMs on my team (in the order their responses appear here): Samara Anjelae (SA), Dave Campbell (DC), Nancy Marlowe (NM), Debra Martin (DeM), Marisa Ryan (MR), Joanne Gerber (JG), Ginger Quinlan (GQ), Laura Lynne Jackson (LJ), Doreen Molloy (DoM), and T.L. Nash (TN). When I collected their responses in March 2021, they gave me permission to publicly share their names as part of “a secret media project” I wouldn’t tell them anything about. I had never asked them those questions before. If this essay makes it into the world, I hope the WCRMs are pleasantly surprised to find their responses here. 

 

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).



Sunday, April 24, 2022

Scientific Method: Beischel excerpt #7

Julie Beischel writes in “Beyond Reasonable: Scientific Evidence for Survival,” her prize-winning essay in the Bigelow Institute for Consciousness Studies competition:


The scientific method is clear. Step 1: observe. Step 2: Formulate a Hypothesis

After observing mediums make statements about discarnates, I can ask, “Are those statements correct? Do they accurately reflect reality?” To develop a hypothesis, I created a positive statement about mediums, discarnates, and sitters predicting reality and based on my observations. The statement needed to be falsifiable: that is, it must be possible for the assertion it makes to be refuted with evidence. The hypothesis was: The information about discarnates reported by mediums is accurate and specific. It is possible for that falsifiable hypothesis to be disproven. Again, I didn’t expect 100% perfection. And rather than use arbitrary assumptions about what would be impressive, I used established statistical methods to objectively determine if the information was actually accurate and specific. Testing the hypothesis allowed me to acquire new knowledge about an aspect of nature. That is the aim of science.

Conjecturing about a phenomenon and then performing experiments based on assumptions does not qualify as science. Any direction starting with, “I wonder if a medium could...” is most likely not a scientific pursuit. I can’t wonder if mediums can report lottery numbers and then ask them to buy a ticket and call that science. That’s not something that they regularly do, and thus it can’t be observed during Step 1. If one observes the mediumistic triad as it exists in nature and then thinks, “Well, then mediums should also be able to [blank]” without ever observing them [blank]ing, no science is happening.

Step 3: Design an Experiment

From the beginning, I knew that in order to appropriately test the hypothesis that the information about discarnates reported by mediums is accurate and specific, two equally important factors of the experiment were necessary. Ideally, laboratory-based mediumship research should include: (a) a research environment that optimizes the mediumship process for both the medium and the discarnate and (b) research methods that maximize the experimental blinding of the medium, the rater, and the experimenters in order to eliminate all conventional explanations for the reported information and its accuracy and specificity.

Without these factors in place, we really won’t know anything more about mediumship after the experiment than we did before it so any results would be meaningless. I have used different analogies to demonstrate this point over the years:

  • You can’t study football on a baseball field using hockey equipment and the rules for soccer and then claim you’ve disproven the existence of football.

  • You can’t place an acorn in your palm, wait a few minutes, and then call it a fraud when it doesn’t turn into an oak tree.

Optimal Environment

To bring an observed natural phenomenon into the laboratory for examination, creating an environment as close to the natural one as possible is necessary. When the phenomenon being studied involves people and not just seeds or chemicals, the research design must include the real-world, lived experiences of the people. In order to acquire new knowledge, my practice has always included bringing research participants into the conversation when designing a study. This allows me to collect feedback about what is and is not their lived experience of the phenomenon under investigation. In the observed phenomenon of mediumship readings, mediums report discarnate-associated information to the discarnates’ living loved ones, the sitters. Therefore, the experimental protocol design needed to account for all three people and their relationships to each other—discarnates, sitters, and mediums—and include reasonable reading conditions. 

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).

Saturday, April 23, 2022

Making an observation: Beischel excerpt #6

Julie Beischel writes in “Beyond Reasonable: Scientific Evidence for Survival,” her prize-winning essay in the Bigelow Institute for Consciousness Studies competition:

What happens after we die is a tremendously important question that speaks to the nature of consciousness, the potentially infinite essence of our relationships to each other, and even the purpose of our existence. The scientific investigation of mediumship allows us to at least peek through the cracks, to get a glimpse of what might be going on after we die. As such, engaging in the scientific method to examine mediumship must be a precise, careful endeavor. What follows covers specific details of my research. In my descriptions, it is necessary to be thorough in order to demonstrate the quality of the evidence I collected. I only included what was directly relevant. What I will share here may seem nearly overwhelming at times but understanding the details of the methods used and the analyses performed is necessary in order to objectively assess the validity of my conclusions. (Here we go. Buckle up!

Using the steps of the scientific method, I can first make an observation about some aspect of nature. I can then formulate a hypothesis about the observed phenomenon. Experiments can then be designed and performed, collected data can be analyzed, and conclusions can be drawn. Then I can start again based on what I learned in the previous cycle. This standard method can easily be applied to studying mediumship.

The phenomenon of mediumship has several advantages that make it an ideal candidate for scientific inquiry in order to gather the best evidence for the survival of human consciousness after permanent bodily death. A relatively plentiful population exists capable of performing the task. These individuals can intentionally engage in the phenomenon and can follow instructions while they do so. The bodily death of the discarnate in a mediumship reading is permanent rather than temporary as is the case for NDEs. We don’t have to wait for the phenomenon to spontaneously happen as is the case for OBEs, for NDEs, and for children who report memories of previous lives. It doesn’t require expensive equipment or a specific laboratory set-up; this makes it possible for the experiments to be easily replicated by other qualified researchers to determine if published findings appropriately reflect the actual phenomenon. We can repeatedly bring mediums into a controlled laboratory environment. This allows us to address aspects of the phenomenon that might complicate what we could conclude if we were to let it just run amok or only observe it spontaneously out in the wild.

Step 1: Make an Observation

It is important for scientists to work from a place of observation. To complete this step, I directly observed that there are people here in the US and here in the 21st century who identify as mediums. I further observed that, as the primary aspect of their mediumship, these modern American mediums verbally utter words, phrases, and sentences during a process called a reading. I observed that those utterances are requested by and provided to a second living person called a sitter. I observed that the content of the reading centers around a third person who is deceased and who we call, during research, a discarnate (dis = not, carnate = in the flesh). The word simply identifies the person as someone who previously existed associated with a physical living body but who is now deceased. It does not imply anything further about the survival, location, or characteristics of that person. It just allows researchers to refer to the three people involved in the reading: the medium, the sitter, and the discarnate.

So far, I don’t think even the hardiest of deniers (often called skeptics) could refute the content of those observations. It is irrefutably true that mediums exist and utter words about discarnates to sitters. Therefore, we cannot move forward in the scientific method without keeping in mind the medium-discarnate-sitter triad that we have observed.

Another observation is that it does not appear to be a flawless connection. There seems to be noise or static in the metaphorical signal. Not every single statement made by a medium resonates with the sitter. That is the reality of the observed phenomenon. It is important that we not expect perfection.

A final observation is that the three people in the mediumship triad are just regular folks. The most evidential info comes from mediumship readings for everyday people containing everyday info that can be objectively verified. I observed that the information mediums most often report falls into three main categories (39, also 40). The first is identifying information that allows the sitter to recognize the discarnate. This usually includes the discarnate’s physical and personality descriptions, favorite activities, and cause of death. The second type of information references events that have happened in the sitter’s life since the death. And the third type of information reported in a mediumship reading involves messages specifically for the sitter. These are the types of statements that you might say to someone with whom you had a close relationship but who had to move away: ‘Thank you for everything’ and ‘I love you.’ More specific messages might encourage, reprimand, or provide advice to the sitter. 

 

So, mediumship readings are not perfect and involve regular information from regular people. We don’t need famous dead people to test mediumship scientifically. In fact, that would be problematic because we couldn’t control for information that could be obtained through normal means like Googling. We don’t need to ask for the secrets to the universe channeled from etheric entities. That would also be problematic because that information could not be objectively verified. All we need are some regular mediums providing regular readings to regular sitters about regular discarnates. Easy peasy.


Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).

Friday, April 22, 2022

Applying science to research: Beischel excerpt #5

Julie Beischel writes in “Beyond Reasonable: Scientific Evidence for Survival,” her prize-winning essay in the Bigelow Institute for Consciousness Studies competition:

Science is considered the most valid and reliable method for acquiring knowledge. It combines the methods of inference and of experience to collect verifiable evidence for natural phenomena. Our Western society and culture require the objective, agreed-upon standards of science to determine what is real. Because people have already made such scientific discoveries as the laws of thermodynamics, the layout of the solar system, and the relationships of chemicals as clarified on the periodic table of elements, we currently use science to know facts like which pharmaceuticals are relatively efficacious and safe and which weather patterns are on their way to our location.

I have long supported the position that science should not have limitations. At the Windbridge Research Center, where I serve as Director of Research, we understand science as simply a set of tools for answering questions. We have found that those tools can be applied to nearly any topic, even a controversial one like life after death. Competent scientists follow the data wherever they lead and do not make unfounded assumptions about what is possible or about how the world works. Assuming that we fully understand every phenomenon in the universe is illogical. True science leaves room for discoveries. Scientists, ideally, just follow the data, draw conclusions, and develop theories. Through science, knowledge is ever evolving.

Viewing science as a widely applicable equal opportunist is not standard. Currently, phenomena considered metaphysical, like mind or spirit, are usually specifically called out as beyond the bounds of science. Some academic sources list metaphysical knowledge gained through various world traditions as important but clarify that “material explanations for observable phenomena are always sufficient and metaphysical explanations are never needed”.

Some sources go even further. One research methods textbook I came across had this to say about the topic of this essay:

Science always investigates empirically solvable problems—questions that are potentially answerable by means of currently available research techniques. If a theory cannot be tested using empirical techniques, then scientists are not interested in it. For example, the question “Is there life after death?” is not an empirical question and thus cannot be tested scientifically.

I beg to differ. No, that’s incorrect. More accurately: I forcefully disagree, with vehemence. Again, science can be used to learn about nearly anything. Also: “cannot be tested scientifically”? Challenge accepted.

As I have previously noted elsewhere, in the Western world, phenomena not easily explained by the traditional, established sciences are usually dismissed as impossible. As a result, people who believe in phenomena like mediumship are labeled ignorant, gullible, or delusional, and the unfortunate individuals who experience mediumistic communication are called frauds, con artists, schizophrenics, evil, or worse. Now, what if we calmed down, put aside our assumptions about how the world works, and actually applied the scientific method to the phenomenon of mediumship? Well, I did just that.

In an effort to evoke your knowing by authority, I will provide my credentials for studying mediums here. After receiving my PhD in 2003, I served as the William James Post- doctoral Fellow in Mediumship and Survival Research in the Department of Psychology at the University of Arizona. I went on, with my husband and research partner, Mark Boccuzzi, to co-found the Windbridge Institute, LLC, in 2008, and then the Windbridge Research Center non- profit in 2017, in order to continue addressing the survival of consciousness hypothesis. I have received multiple mediumship research grants from international funding foundations, have shared my findings at various conferences through juried and invited presentations, and published my work performing controlled laboratory research with mediums in several peer-reviewed journals.

 

Dr. Julie Beischel is the Director of Research at the Windbridge Research Center. She received her PhD in Pharmacology and Toxicology with a minor in Microbiology and Immunology from the University of Arizona and uses her interdisciplinary training to apply the scientific method to controversial topics. For over 15 years, Dr. Beischel has worked full-time studying mediums: individuals who report experiencing communication with the deceased and who regularly, reliably, and on-demand report the specific resulting messages to the living. References cited in her paper are deleted from these excerpts but a full paper with references is available at the Bigelow website (https://bigelowinstitute.org/contest_winners3.php).


Gödel's reasons for an afterlife

Alexander T. Englert, “We'll meet again,” Aeon , Jan 2, 2024, https://aeon.co/essays/kurt-godel-his-mother-and-the-a...